Difference between revisions of "Distraction"
From OptimalScience
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
− | Key claims | + | == Key claims == |
*[[A distraction can be defined as an urge to avoid the present task in order to reduce the effort required]]. | *[[A distraction can be defined as an urge to avoid the present task in order to reduce the effort required]]. |
Revision as of 14:24, 20 May 2020
Key claims
- A distraction can be defined as an urge to avoid the present task in order to reduce the effort required.
- Distracting impulses are also subject to sensitization and habituation.
- Distractibility is not a permanent trait.
- Distractibility is a state of training.
- Some people develop certain habits with less training than others.
- Learning to feel the urge of the distracting impulse makes habituation easier.
- Emotions are best processed when felt in the chest.
- Traction in a task is when your level of engagement for the task matches the effort required.
- With enough engagement, you attain flow.
- Distraction occurs when you try to lower the effort the task requires, which produces disengagement.
- Disengagement from the task, coupled with high effort required, is seen in states of hyperfocus and automation.
- Feeling an emotion or urge sends a “message received” signal through the same vagus nerve back to your amygdala.
- The most crucial distinction within types of attention is task attention vs. predictive (or default) attention.
- Task attention uses your central executive function network.
- Predictive attention, also called default attention, uses the default mode network.
- Task attention works in the present moment.
- Predictive attention concerns the past or future.
- The main content of predictive attention is unfinished business.
- Traction comes from these two modes of attention working together.
- This working together of the two modes of attention produces flow.
- We have deliberate and automated appraisals.
- We also have deliberate (task) and automated (predictive) attention.